As scientists delve deeper into the realms of biotechnology, an intriguing yet controversial question looms: Could lab-grown brains ever achieve consciousness? Kenneth Kosik, a neuroscientist from the University of California at Santa Barbara, has recently brought this existential conundrum to the forefront. Despite the groundbreaking advances in creating brain organoids, Kosik remains skeptical about their potential to become truly conscious entities.
Brain organoids, often misleadingly termed “minibrains,” are developed by converting human cells into stem cells and subsequently differentiating them into neurons. The magic happens when these neurons are submerged into a substance known as “Matrigel.” Depending on the temperature, Matrigel can be a liquid or a solid, allowing the neurons to expand into three dimensions rather than just two. This seemingly simple process results in tiny brain-like structures that resemble human brains in tissue composition and electrical activity. However, Kosik is quick to emphasize the limitations of these structures.
Kosik argues that calling these organoids “minibrains” is a significant misnomer. While they may exhibit a loose resemblance to actual brains, they lack the intricate complexity required for consciousness. In a recent perspective article published in the journal Cell, Kosik elaborates on this point. Despite their uncanny resemblance to miniaturized brains, these organoids are far from developing the sophisticated capabilities needed for consciousness. They might sprout tissues and emit electrical signals, but this is a far cry from achieving the cognitive functions associated with being truly conscious.
One of the remarkable aspects of brain organoids is their potential in the field of “biocomputing.” Scientists have managed to connect these organoids to act as processors, opening new avenues for research and technology. Despite these advances, Kosik remains cautious. It is still unclear whether these brain organoids can store or process information in the way that human brains can. Furthermore, even if they could, scientists would face the additional challenge of figuring out how to transmit human-level information to these organoids effectively.
Kosik provides a vivid analogy to explain this limitation. He describes brain organoids as vehicles capable of encoding experience and information, but notes that they currently lack the sensory inputs to make this possible. Without eyes, ears, nose, or mouth, these organoids have no means of receiving external stimuli. Thus, while they have the potential for spontaneous neuronal organization, they are devoid of the experiences needed to encode meaningful information. Essentially, they are like sophisticated machines waiting for an input that never arrives.
In summary, the world of brain organoids presents exciting possibilities but also significant limitations. As researchers continue to make strides in biotechnology, the question of consciousness in lab-grown brains remains an open and highly debatable topic. For now, as Kosik aptly puts it, these organoids are fascinating but far from evolving into conscious entities. They might resemble miniaturized brains, but the journey from mimicking brain tissue to achieving consciousness is a monumental leap that science has yet to make.